The Institute of Canine Biology
  • HOME
  • Blog
  • Courses
    • COI BootCamp (FREE!)
    • Basic Population Genetics (FREE)
    • The Science of Canine Husbandry
    • Managing Genetics For the Future >
      • Syllabus - Managing Genetics for the Future
    • The Biology of Dogs (Open Reg )
    • DNA For Dog Breeders >
      • Syllabus - DNA for Dog Breeders
      • Open Reg - DNA For Dog Breeders
    • Understanding Hip & Elbow Dysplasia >
      • Open Reg - Understanding Hip & Elbow Dysplasia
    • Genetics of Behavior & Performance >
      • Syllabus - Genetics Behavior & Performance
      • Open Reg - Genetics of Behavior & Performance (Open Reg)
    • Strategies for Preservation Breeding >
      • Open Reg - Strategies for Preservation Breeding
    • Group Discounts
    • MORE FREE COURSES >
      • Quickie Genetics (Free!)
      • Heredity & Genetics (Free!)
      • Useful Genetics (Free!)
      • Basic Genetics Videos
  • Breed Preservation
    • Breed Status
    • Breeding for the future >
      • BFF Breed Groups
    • The "Elevator Pitch"
    • What's in the Gene Pool?
    • The Pox of Popular Sires
    • What population genetics can tell us about a breed
    • What population genetics can tell you...Tollers & Heelers
    • How to use kinship data
    • Using EBVs to breed better dogs >
      • How population size affects inbreeding
      • EBV Examples
    • How to read a dendrogram
    • Global Pedigree Project >
      • The Database
    • Finding the genes without DNA
    • How to read a heat map
  • Health Data
    • Bloat (Purdue Study)
    • Body Condition Score >
      • % Dysplastic vs BCS
    • Breed Comparions
    • Cancer
    • Cardiac
    • Cataracts
    • Caesareans
    • Deafness
    • Degenerative Myelopathy
    • Elbow Dysplasia
    • Epilepsy
    • Genetic Diversity
    • Genetic Diversity (MyDogDNA)
    • Hip Dysplasia >
      • Hip Dysplasia (Hou et al 2013)
    • Inbreeding Effects
    • Inbreeding (Gubbels)
    • Inbreeding (Dreger)
    • Lifespan
    • Litter size
    • Metabolic
    • mtDNA
    • Orthopedic
    • Mode of Inheritance
    • Patella Luxation
    • Thyroid
    • Portosystemic shunt
    • Purebred vs Mixed (UC Davis)
    • Purebred vs Mixed Breed (Bonnett)
    • Spay & Neuter Effects
    • Calboli et al 2008
    • Hodgman (1963)
    • Scott & Fuller (1965)
    • Stockard: Purebred crosses
    • Summers (2011)
  • Projects
    • How To Interpret Breed Analyses
    • Afghan Hound
    • More details about the Toller study
    • Belgian Tervuren >
      • Belgian Terv p2
      • Belgians- why population size matters
    • Bernese Mountain Dog
    • Boxer
    • Brussels Griffon
    • Bullmastiff
    • Canaan Dog >
      • Canaan analyses
    • Cesky Terrier >
      • Cesky genetic history
    • Chinook
    • Curly-coated Retriever
    • Doberman
    • Entelbucher Mountain Dog
    • Flatcoat Retriever
    • French Bulldog
    • German Shorthair
    • Golden Retriever >
      • Golden Retriever Pedigree Charts
    • Irish Water Spaniel >
      • IWS (6 Nov 17)
    • Labrador Retriever
    • Manchester Terrier
    • Mongolian Bankhar >
      • Research Updates
      • Bankhar 1
    • Norwegian Lundehund
    • Plummer Terrier
    • Otterhound
    • Portuguese Water Dog >
      • Portuguese Water Dog (pt 2)
    • Ridgeback
    • Schipperke
    • Standard Poodle >
      • The Problem With Poodles
      • 3poodle pedigree charts
      • 3Poodle Wycliff dogs
      • Poodle Genetics
    • Tibetan Spaniel
    • Tibetan Mastiff
    • West Highland White Terrier
    • Whippet
    • Wirehaired Pointing Griffons
    • UK KC Graphs >
      • UK KC Breed Status
      • UK Groups
      • KC Gundogs
      • KC Hounds
      • KC Terriers >
        • Terriers (select breeds)
      • KC Pastoral
      • KC Toys
      • KC Working
      • KC Utility
      • Australian KC
    • Breed outcrossing programs
  • Resources
    • Genetics Databases
    • Stud Books >
      • American Kennel Club stud books
      • Field Dog stud books
      • The Kennel Club (UK)
    • Learn
    • Videos about dog genetics
    • The Amazing Things Dogs Do! (videos) >
      • Livestock Management
      • Livestock guarding
      • Transportation, exploration, racing
      • Conservation & wildlife management
      • Detection Dogs
      • Medicine & Research
      • Entertainment
      • AKC/CHF Podcasts
    • Read & Watch
    • Bookshelf
  • Preventing Uterine Inertia

The genetics of canine behavior goes molecular

2/27/2019

 
By Carol Beuchat PhD

Before you decide what this paper is about, please read all of it, including my explanation at the very bottom. It's probably not about what you think it is.
​

It would be fair to say that the pioneering studies conducted by Scott and Fuller using dogs in the 1950s and 1960s gave birth to the modern scientific discipline of behavioral genetics. It has only been in the last decade, however, that dogs have again caught the interest of biologists interested in the evolution and domestication of dogs, and once again research on dogs is pushing forward the fields of animal behavior and cognition.

With the availability of affordable DNA analysis that can provide information about hundreds of thousands - or even millions - of markers, we have started to look for the genetic basis of behavior at the level of the gene. A few genes have been identified that are associated with specific behavioral traits in dogs, like xxxxxxxx (Bridgett). There is also a growing number of studies that looked for the genetic basis of behavioral differences in dog. But as I explained in an earlier post, the associations of behavior with genes have been generally weak and not especially useful for identifying genes of major effect or improving traits through genomic selection. 

A new study, however, is a game-changer (MacLean et al 2019). It has been made available before submission for publication, so it has not been subject to peer review and you should keep that in mind. But what it offers is a tantalizing first look at links between genes and behavior in dogs. 
Previous studies have looked for variation in genes that could account for differences in behavior in a group of dogs. The strength of the association between genetics and a trait is indicated by a statistic called "heritability", and in most studies of behavioral traits in dogs of a particular breed, the heritability was found to be low; i.e., variations in behavior were not associated with genetics.

Instead of focusing on a particular breed, the new study looked for major differences in behavior and genetics across many breeds. And here, they were successful.
In this study, a large set of behavioral assessments from CBARQ testing of > 17,000 dogs was paired with DNA genotyping data for > 5,600 dogs of 141 breeds  by combining information from two separate studies (Hayward et al. and Parker et al.) that used the current gold-standard analysis platform (high density SNP markers; > 100,000). They identified 14 behavioral traits of interest and looked for differences in the behavior of breeds that appeared to be associated with differences in the DNA, which would tell them the heritability of these specific traits.

​What they found was fascinating.


In this graph, the behaviors they looked at are indicated down the y-axis, and the strength of the association between genetics and the trait, i.e., heritability, is on the x-axis. The stronger the association, the higher the heritability. The grey dogs are measurements of heritability made on dogs of the same breed. The green and yellow dogs display heritability of those same traits when the associations are made looking across breeds.

You can see here that heritabilities determined from dogs of the same breed are generally low, less than about 0.3, which means that 30% or less of the variations in behavior can be attributed to variations in genotype.

But when you compare across breeds, the average heritabilities are high, from about 0.4 (40% of variation explained) to more than 0.7 (70% of variation explained.

Picture
This is very exciting, because it's the first time we have been able to detect strong links between genes and behavior. 

This is a game-changer. Our understanding of the genetic basis of behavior in dogs has gone molecular.

We now have large databases for both behavior and genotype, and these will continue to grow in size because the methodology for both is standardized (CBARQ for behavior and high-density SNP for genotype). These allow us to do analyses like the one below, which is a "heat map" that depicts the behavioral scores by breed displayed along with the dendrogram displaying their genetic relatedness. You can easily find the sporting dogs because they are high in trainability, and the small breeds like the chihuahua, rat terrier, and miniature pinscher stand out for scoring  high in traits that we might describe as reactive or "fiesty" (e.g., aggression, fear). The herding breeds score high in trainability while the hounds are low. And now we are finally able to explore the genetic basis for major breed-specific differences in behavioral traits among dog breeds. 
​
Picture

This paper will need to go through peer review, revision if necessary, then publication, so don't expect to see the final version for at least a few months. But I suspect we're about to see a flood of studies that also leverage the large datasets now available for both behavior and genetics. Buckle up!

NOTE: If you are interested in the links between genetics and behavior, now is the time to start boning up on the jargon and concepts you need to understand these studies. You can find relevant articles in the blog posts on the ICB website (just search for a topic or keyword). Even better, ICB has courses designed for dog people with no background in science that will get you up to speed. This genie is out of the bottle. A little homework now to learn the basics will pay off every time you read about an exciting new paper about the genetics of behavior in dogs!
Coincidently, the ICB course "​The Genetics of Behavior & Performance in Dogs" starts next week, 4 March 2019. We will be covering this and other new studies, so you'll be up to date on the latest information. This would be a great time to take it!

NOTE added after posting:
People are drawing all sorts of conclusions about behavior of dog breeds from this paper. That's not what this paper is about. 

Here it is in a nutshell:


For 14 traits in dog breeds, in general

h ^2 (heritability) = vg/(vp) = 0.4 to 0.7

where vp = vg + ve.

What is so ground-breaking about the study is that the estimates of h ^2 across breeds are much greater than h ^2 determined within breeds.


Wow. What on earth is that?

This is not a paper about behavior. It is about measuring something called heritability (h ^2). Here's the translation of that first equation:

Heritability is the fraction of the variation in a trait that can be attributed to variation in genetics.

This study shows that there is a measurable fraction of the variation in a number of behavioral traits in dogs that is due to genetics. 

Heritability doesn't mean "inherited". It doesn't tell you how much variation there is in a trait. It doesn't tell you how likely a dog is to inherit a behavior. None of those things.

Clearly, to understand this paper, you need to be really clear on the meaning of heritability. You can start learning about it by reading some of my blogs on the ICB website, starting with this one:

                Understanding the heritability of behavior in dogs             

There are others if you do a search.

Also, please consider taking the Genetics of Behavior course. We will go through some papers like this one (including this one!) so you have a good understanding of what the information does - and doesn't - mean. There will be more studies like this one. Make sure you get the most out of them by understanding the science.

Check out those blog posts, and consider taking my course that starts (coincidentally) next week, 4 March 2019:

REFERENCES

MacLean EL, N Snyder-Mackler, BM vonHoldt, & JA Serpell. (unpub) Highly heritable and functionally relevant breed differences in dog behavior. BioRxiv preprint accessed 27 February 2019.. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/509315.

To learn more about the genetics of dogs, check out
ICB's online courses

***************************************

Visit our Facebook Groups

ICB Institute of Canine Biology
...the latest canine news and research

ICB Breeding for the Future
...the science of animal breeding

The messy science of assessing working ability in dogs

2/14/2019

 
By Carol Beuchat PhD
Dealing with the behavioral issues of dogs is a big business. Trainers and therapists abound, as do the professional suffixes tagged after the names of those who took some sort of course or were accredited by an organization.

I have no idea whether the majority of these people know what they're doing. I can tell you, though, that the scientific literature about behavior in dogs is a morass of tangled, insufficient, and often contradictory studies. In fact, in the field of canine behavior, science is marching forward very slowly, even as the ranks of lay behaviorists swell and practitioners claim to become ever more authoritative and confident of their expertise.
How reliable are behavioral assessments of dogs? Are they repeatable, meaningful, and verifiable? This especially matters when dogs are being assessed for working ability, as in breeding programs for service, police, and other types of working dogs. Breeding and training these dogs is time consuming and expensive, and being able to accurately assess the usefulness of a potential working animal is the differences between success and failure.

A recent study evaluated the reliability and validity of various behavioral tests
that are used to assess the traits of working dogs (Brady et al 2018). What the researchers found should give pause to anybody that professes to have a deep and true understanding of canine behavior. 
The aim of this study was to assess the reliability and predictive validity of tests designed to assess relevant traits of working dogs. They reviewed many published, peer-reviewed studies and proceeded to evaluate only those that met strict criteria for focus, design, methods, and analysis. Of the thousands of behavior studies they reviewed, 16 were judged to meet the requirements of their analysis.
​

Picture

For the selected studies, they evaluated the reliability, repeatability, and predictive validity of the behavioral tests,  both within and among "raters" (the individuals conducting the evaluation). They had an assortment of problems to deal with, like the use of different terminology for the same trait. More seriously, they found that data were often inadequately reported, statistical analyses were misinterpreted, and there was an overall lack of concordance in the various methods used for assessment.

​Ultimately, they concluded that there is "a widespread lack of information relating to the reliability and validity of measures to assess behaviour and inconsistencies in terminologies, study parameters and indices of success...This review indicates that we are still not addressing concerns over the lack of standardisation (sic) amongst research on dog behavioural tests". 
What this means is that we are probably not as good at assessing behavioral traits as we think we are or, at the very least, it's hard to tell how good we are because of limitations of the studies and the lack of standardization in terminology and protocol. Certainly, it should give pause to anybody relying on "experts" for advice, most of whom cannot lean on published studies to support claims about the validity and reliability of their assessments.

Buyer beware.
You can download a copy of the study below.

REFERENCES

Brady, K., N. Cracknell, H. Zulch, and D.S. Mills. 2018. A systematic review of the reliability and validity of behavioural tests used to assess behavioural characteristics important in working dogs. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 5(103);doi: 10.3389/fvets.2018.00103.

Are you interested in dog behavior?
​

​You can learn about the genetic basis of behavior in dogs in ICB's online course.


The Genetics of Behavior & Performance​
Online course starts 4 March 2019
​10 weeks, $125

Picture
Learn more

To learn more about the genetics of dogs, check out
ICB's online courses

***************************************

Visit our Facebook Groups

ICB Institute of Canine Biology
...the latest canine news and research

ICB Breeding for the Future
...the science of animal breeding

A new ICB course that will use the DNA data from YOUR dog!

2/5/2019

 
By Carol Beuchat PhD
​
Have you ever wondered how scientists figure out the genetic basis of a particular trait? This is especially difficult if the trait is polygenic and many genes plus environmental effects might be involved.

These are called "continuous" or quantitative traits; most of the traits under selection by dog breeders are of this type. For most of us, the process of identifying the markers associated with particular traits is a "black box" from which information mysteriously emerges from huge files of cryptic genotype data. How are scientists able to identify which of the many of thousands of markers play a role in the expression of a specific trait? ​​

Picture
In ICB's has a new course, The Genetics of Continuous Traits, we'll take the lid off the box and show you how scientists get from genotype to phenotype using DNA data you provide for your own dogs! For a learning example, we will study the genetics of body size, which is a trait that varies widely and continuously among breeds and mixed-breed dogs.

​We will use the raw DNA data file provided by Embark for your own dog, together with information about the size of your dog that you will provide. If we get data for enough dogs of a single breed, we can also explore the genetic basis of variation within a breed, also for a variety of mixed-breed dogs.

We will review some published studies for comparison with our own results, so you will also get to make comparisons with "real world" information.

This course is a great opportunity for you explore "under the hood" and learn how scientists go from a table of data to new information extracted from the data for your own dog!

Don't miss this great learning opportunity and the chance to analyze the data for your dog!

This unique online course is $125 and lasts 10 weeks, with students free to work along with the class or at their own pace. There are no prerequisites and a scientific background is not required - just your keen interest in dogs!

Class starts 11 February. Register now to join us!
​

The Genetics of Continuous Traits

Check out the information for the course and register here


https://www.instituteofcaninebiology.org/continuous_traits.html
LEARN MORE

To learn more about the genetics of dogs, check out
ICB's online courses

***************************************

Visit our Facebook Groups

ICB Institute of Canine Biology
...the latest canine news and research

ICB Breeding for the Future
...the science of animal breeding

    Archives

    January 2025
    November 2022
    July 2022
    May 2022
    April 2022
    March 2022
    February 2022
    November 2021
    October 2021
    December 2020
    January 2020
    August 2019
    July 2019
    June 2019
    May 2019
    April 2019
    March 2019
    February 2019
    January 2019
    December 2018
    November 2018
    September 2018
    August 2018
    July 2018
    June 2018
    May 2018
    October 2017
    August 2017
    May 2017
    April 2017
    March 2017
    February 2017
    January 2017
    December 2016
    November 2016
    September 2016
    August 2016
    July 2016
    June 2016
    April 2016
    March 2016
    February 2016
    January 2016
    December 2015
    November 2015
    October 2015
    September 2015
    August 2015
    July 2015
    June 2015
    May 2015
    April 2015
    March 2015
    January 2015
    December 2014
    November 2014
    October 2014
    September 2014
    August 2014
    July 2014
    June 2014
    May 2014
    February 2014
    December 2013
    October 2013
    September 2013
    July 2013
    March 2013
    July 2012
    April 2012

    Categories

    All
    Behavior
    Border-collie
    Herding

Blog

News


About Us

Contact Us








Copyright © 2012-2017 Institute of Canine Biology
Picture
Picture