The Institute of Canine Biology
  • HOME
  • Blog
  • Courses
    • COI BootCamp (FREE!)
    • Basic Population Genetics (FREE)
    • The Science of Canine Husbandry
    • Managing Genetics For the Future >
      • Syllabus - Managing Genetics for the Future
    • The Biology of Dogs (Open Reg )
    • DNA For Dog Breeders >
      • Syllabus - DNA for Dog Breeders
      • Open Reg - DNA For Dog Breeders
    • Understanding Hip & Elbow Dysplasia >
      • Open Reg - Understanding Hip & Elbow Dysplasia
    • Genetics of Behavior & Performance >
      • Syllabus - Genetics Behavior & Performance
      • Open Reg - Genetics of Behavior & Performance (Open Reg)
    • Strategies for Preservation Breeding >
      • Open Reg - Strategies for Preservation Breeding
    • Group Discounts
    • MORE FREE COURSES >
      • Quickie Genetics (Free!)
      • Heredity & Genetics (Free!)
      • Useful Genetics (Free!)
      • Basic Genetics Videos
  • Breed Preservation
    • Breed Status
    • Breeding for the future >
      • BFF Breed Groups
    • The "Elevator Pitch"
    • What's in the Gene Pool?
    • The Pox of Popular Sires
    • What population genetics can tell us about a breed
    • What population genetics can tell you...Tollers & Heelers
    • How to use kinship data
    • Using EBVs to breed better dogs >
      • How population size affects inbreeding
      • EBV Examples
    • How to read a dendrogram
    • Global Pedigree Project >
      • The Database
    • Finding the genes without DNA
    • How to read a heat map
  • Health Data
    • Bloat (Purdue Study)
    • Body Condition Score >
      • % Dysplastic vs BCS
    • Breed Comparions
    • Cancer
    • Cardiac
    • Cataracts
    • Caesareans
    • Deafness
    • Degenerative Myelopathy
    • Elbow Dysplasia
    • Epilepsy
    • Genetic Diversity
    • Genetic Diversity (MyDogDNA)
    • Hip Dysplasia >
      • Hip Dysplasia (Hou et al 2013)
    • Inbreeding Effects
    • Inbreeding (Gubbels)
    • Inbreeding (Dreger)
    • Lifespan
    • Litter size
    • Metabolic
    • mtDNA
    • Orthopedic
    • Mode of Inheritance
    • Patella Luxation
    • Thyroid
    • Portosystemic shunt
    • Purebred vs Mixed (UC Davis)
    • Purebred vs Mixed Breed (Bonnett)
    • Spay & Neuter Effects
    • Calboli et al 2008
    • Hodgman (1963)
    • Scott & Fuller (1965)
    • Stockard: Purebred crosses
    • Summers (2011)
  • Projects
    • How To Interpret Breed Analyses
    • Afghan Hound
    • More details about the Toller study
    • Belgian Tervuren >
      • Belgian Terv p2
      • Belgians- why population size matters
    • Bernese Mountain Dog
    • Boxer
    • Brussels Griffon
    • Bullmastiff
    • Canaan Dog >
      • Canaan analyses
    • Cesky Terrier >
      • Cesky genetic history
    • Chinook
    • Curly-coated Retriever
    • Doberman
    • Entelbucher Mountain Dog
    • Flatcoat Retriever
    • French Bulldog
    • German Shorthair
    • Golden Retriever >
      • Golden Retriever Pedigree Charts
    • Irish Water Spaniel >
      • IWS (6 Nov 17)
    • Labrador Retriever
    • Manchester Terrier
    • Mongolian Bankhar >
      • Research Updates
      • Bankhar 1
    • Norwegian Lundehund
    • Plummer Terrier
    • Otterhound
    • Portuguese Water Dog >
      • Portuguese Water Dog (pt 2)
    • Ridgeback
    • Schipperke
    • Standard Poodle >
      • The Problem With Poodles
      • 3poodle pedigree charts
      • 3Poodle Wycliff dogs
      • Poodle Genetics
    • Tibetan Spaniel
    • Tibetan Mastiff
    • West Highland White Terrier
    • Whippet
    • Wirehaired Pointing Griffons
    • UK KC Graphs >
      • UK KC Breed Status
      • UK Groups
      • KC Gundogs
      • KC Hounds
      • KC Terriers >
        • Terriers (select breeds)
      • KC Pastoral
      • KC Toys
      • KC Working
      • KC Utility
      • Australian KC
    • Breed outcrossing programs
  • Resources
    • Genetics Databases
    • Stud Books >
      • American Kennel Club stud books
      • Field Dog stud books
      • The Kennel Club (UK)
    • Learn
    • Videos about dog genetics
    • The Amazing Things Dogs Do! (videos) >
      • Livestock Management
      • Livestock guarding
      • Transportation, exploration, racing
      • Conservation & wildlife management
      • Detection Dogs
      • Medicine & Research
      • Entertainment
      • AKC/CHF Podcasts
    • Read & Watch
    • Bookshelf
  • Preventing Uterine Inertia

If knowledge is power, know every puppy

3/24/2015

 
By Carol Beuchat PhD

Building a dog is complicated. For some traits (and many genetic disorders) only a single gene is involved. But for most, the phenotype of the animal depends on a genotype that involves many genes. Many of these so-called polygenic traits can vary along continuous scale of expression. Some good examples would be height, running speed, and intelligence.

Polygenic traits can be especially difficult to select for (or against) because their expression is often affected by both genes and environment. For example, puppies raised with restricted food might not grow as large as their siblings, and dogs that are less fit might not be able to run as fast as those with performance training. 
Picture
These non-genetic influences on the expression of phenotype are collectively called "environmental" factors. For may inherited traits, the phenotype you observe is a consequence of both genetic and and environmental influences.
Phenotype = Genetics + Environment
or
P = G + E
When you're evaluating a trait in a dog, you need to remember that some of the variation you observe among individuals, and even among litter mates, will be the result of genetics and some will be a consequence non-genetic or environmental factors. For example, a dog might have great genes for hips but a bad hip score because it was over-exercised as a puppy. If you're trying to improve the hips in your offspring, you would want to know the difference between dogs with bad hips because of environment and those with bad hips because they have the genes for bad hips. One would be safe to breed from, the other wouldn't. What you need to know is the "heritability" of that trait.

Heritability is the fraction of the variability in a trait among animals that can be accounted for by genetics; that is, we're looking at the G in the equation above. Let's say you raise an entire litter of puppies under conditions that are identical in every way. If the "environment" term in our equation is zero (i.e., there was no effect of environment), then the heritability of that trait in those puppies is 100%; all of the variation among puppies can be accounted for by differences in genetics. On the other hand, you might measure the same trait in 300 puppies raised in different households. The environment in this case will make a difference and will be different for each litter (and maybe different for each puppy), so genetics will account for less of the variation you see from pup to pup. Heritability is not about how a trait is inherited, but about how much of the variation in a trait from dog to dog an be accounted for by genetics.

Because every group of dogs could experience different environmental factors, an estimate of heritability is specific for a particular group of animals measured at a particular time. If a trait has been measured on many groups of animals under different circumstances, it's possible to determine an average or a range of heritability values that can be used to characterize that trait in general. So for example, estimates of the heritability of subaortic stenosis in the Newfoundland range from 0.17 to 0.45. Heritabilty of lymphoma in Golden Retrievers is 0.44, and of osteosarcoma in Deerhounds is 0.7. Behavioral traits are also heritable: heritability of various hunting traits in Finnish Hounds range from 0.12 to 0.15. (See the table HERE for heritability estimates of various traits.)

It's important to understand heritability because traits with low heritability are more difficult to improve by selection. The phenotype of the trait less reliably reflects the genotype of the dog (so it's harder to select the dogs with the best genes), plus the genes account for less of the improvement you would gain from selection. It is easier to improve traits with high heritability, and you could waste a lot of time and effort trying to improve through selection a trait that is strongly influenced by non-genetic factors.

If you have a rough idea of the heritability of a trait and it's high enough to respond well to selection, then what you wish you knew were the actual genes in a particular dog. A trait might have good heritability, but you won't get very far if the dog you want to breed doesn't have the genes you're trying to select for.

Of course, your best sources of information about the genes in a particular dog are that dog's relatives - the parents it inherited the genes from, the siblings that would also carry some of the same genes, and the progeny that the genes would be passed to. This is where long-time breeders have a definite advantage; knowing the history of a particular line, knowing the parents, grandparents, aunts and uncles, and knowing what a dog produced are all clues to the genetics packaged inside a particular dog. If you don't have this wealth of experience, or you don't know much about a particular line, what information would be most useful for you to collect?

These are graphs that display the relationship between how reliable a particular group of relatives would be in selecting for a particular trait and how this varies with the heritability of the trait. [In these graphs, the environmental effects are not a factor.  Reliability is the relationship (as a percentage) between the genetic merit of the dog being evaluated and the phenotype being selected for.]

The first graph below shows that the dog itself provides the most reliable information compared to parents, parents plus grandparents, and the complete pedigree. This graph also shows that the higher the heritability of the trait you are selecting for, the more reliable the dog becomes as a predictor of what will be produced in a particular mating. For traits of low to modest heritability like aortic stenosis in Newfoundlands (0.17-0.45), the phenotype of the dog will predict about 45-65% of the dog's actual genetic merit for that trait. Knowing that a Deerhound suffers from osteosarcoma (heritability of 0.7) would allow you to predict the dog's genotype for that trait with a reliability of about 85%, whereas having that information about parents or grandparents is much less reliable (about 60%). So the information about the dog itself is most informative, and the higher the heritability the more reliable it is. (These data about heritability are from the table in "A better way to pick 'em: using EBVs to reduce genetic disorders in dogs".)

Picture

What about information about siblings and half-siblings? These two graphs show how reliability varies if you have information about 2, 4, or 6 full siblings or half-sibs.  As you might expect, the more littermates and half-sibs you have information about, the more reliable your assessment of a dog's genotype. Also, full siblings are more informative than half-sibs. Two full siblings provide about as much information as parents for a trait with a specific heritability; for instance 2 siblings and parents have a reliability of about 20% for a trait with a heritability of 10. For traits with higher heritability, the parents are a bit more informative. So information about siblings is very useful, and that from half-sibs is modestly so.

Picture
Picture

The most reliable information, however, comes from progeny, each of which carries half the genes of that parent. Again as you would expect, the more offspring you have information about, the more reliably you can estimate the actual genetic merit of a dog for a trait, and the gain from additional offspring is most pronounced for traits with low heritability. This graph depicts reliability for 10, 20, 40, 80, and 120 puppies (the legend is in the lower left corner), and for traits with high heritability puppies are the most reliable reflection of the genetic merit of a dog. So for traits with low heritability, the more information you can gather about the trait in offspring the better; reliability of selection improves by about 50% if you have information for 40 puppies instead of just 10, and reliability almost doubles with 80 puppies.
Picture
The take home lesson here is that having information about the progeny of a dog is extremely useful in the assessment of a dog's genetic merit for a particular trait. Every breeder evaluates their puppies, but how many keep systematic records of the information for every trait of interest and for each puppy? Probably not many, and as the graph just above shows, the ability to assess the merit of a dog can be improved substantially with information about more offspring. For polygenic traits in which assessment of quality based solely on phenotype will be confounded by environmental factors (many of which you might know nothing about), being able to predict more reliably the traits a dog will pass on to offspring will make selection all that more efficient.

Data from -
  • Willis MB. 1989. Genetics of the dog. (table 92)

You can learn more about the genetics of dogs in ICB's Online Courses.

***   Population Genetics for Dog Breeders  ***
Next class starts 30 March 


Visit our Facebook Groups

ICB Institute of Canine Biology
...the latest canine news and research

ICB Breeding for the Future
...the science of animal breeding 

Comments are closed.

    Archives

    January 2025
    November 2022
    July 2022
    May 2022
    April 2022
    March 2022
    February 2022
    November 2021
    October 2021
    December 2020
    January 2020
    August 2019
    July 2019
    June 2019
    May 2019
    April 2019
    March 2019
    February 2019
    January 2019
    December 2018
    November 2018
    September 2018
    August 2018
    July 2018
    June 2018
    May 2018
    October 2017
    August 2017
    May 2017
    April 2017
    March 2017
    February 2017
    January 2017
    December 2016
    November 2016
    September 2016
    August 2016
    July 2016
    June 2016
    April 2016
    March 2016
    February 2016
    January 2016
    December 2015
    November 2015
    October 2015
    September 2015
    August 2015
    July 2015
    June 2015
    May 2015
    April 2015
    March 2015
    January 2015
    December 2014
    November 2014
    October 2014
    September 2014
    August 2014
    July 2014
    June 2014
    May 2014
    February 2014
    December 2013
    October 2013
    September 2013
    July 2013
    March 2013
    July 2012
    April 2012

    Categories

    All
    Behavior
    Border-collie
    Herding

Blog

News


About Us

Contact Us








Copyright © 2012-2017 Institute of Canine Biology
Picture
Picture