The Institute of Canine Biology
  • HOME
  • Blog
  • Breed Preservation
    • Breeding for the future >
      • BFF Breed Groups
    • Breed Status
    • The "Elevator Pitch"
    • What's in the Gene Pool?
    • What population genetics can tell us about a breed
    • What population genetics can tell you...Tollers & Heelers
    • How to use kinship data
    • Using EBVs to breed better dogs >
      • How population size affects inbreeding
      • EBV Examples
    • How to read a dendrogram
    • Global Pedigree Project >
      • The Database
    • Finding the genes without DNA
    • How to read a heat map
  • Courses
    • COI BootCamp (FREE!)
    • Basic Population Genetics (FREE)
    • Genetic rescue: the genetics of cross breeding (NEW!)
    • The Biology of Dogs (Open Reg )
    • Managing Genetics For the Future >
      • Managing Genetics For the Future Open Reg (Open Reg) >
        • Syllabus - Managing Genetics for the Future
    • Genetics of Behavior & Performance (Open Reg)
    • Strategies for Preservation Breeding (Open Reg)
    • DNA For Dog Breeders (May2025) >
      • DNA For Dog Breeders (Open Reg)
    • Understanding Hip & Elbow Dysplasia (Open Reg) >
      • MORE FREE COURSES >
        • Quickie Genetics (Free!)
        • Group Discounts
        • Useful Genetics (Free!)
        • Strategies for Preservation Breeding (Sept 2023) >
          • Heredity & Genetics (Free!)
        • Basic Genetics Videos
  • Health Data
    • Bloat (Purdue Study)
    • Body Condition Score >
      • % Dysplastic vs BCS
    • Breed Comparions
    • Cancer
    • Cardiac
    • Cataracts
    • Caesareans
    • Deafness
    • Degenerative Myelopathy
    • Elbow Dysplasia
    • Epilepsy
    • Genetic Diversity
    • Genetic Diversity (MyDogDNA)
    • Hip Dysplasia >
      • Hip Dysplasia (Hou et al 2013)
    • Metabolic
    • Inbreeding Effects
    • Inbreeding (Gubbels)
    • Inbreeding (Dreger)
    • Lifespan
    • Litter size
    • mtDNA
    • Orthopedic
    • Mode of Inheritance
    • Patella Luxation
    • Thyroid
    • Portosystemic shunt
    • Purebred vs Mixed (UC Davis)
    • Purebred vs Mixed Breed (Bonnett)
    • Spay & Neuter Effects
    • Calboli et al 2008
    • Hodgman (1963)
    • Scott & Fuller (1965)
    • Stockard: Purebred crosses
    • Summers (2011)
  • Projects
    • How To Interpret Breed Analyses
    • Afghan Hound
    • More details about the Toller study
    • Belgian Tervuren >
      • Belgian Terv p2
      • Belgians- why population size matters
    • Bernese Mountain Dog
    • Boxer
    • Brussels Griffon
    • Bullmastiff
    • Canaan Dog >
      • Canaan analyses
    • Cesky Terrier >
      • Cesky genetic history
    • Chinook
    • Curly-coated Retriever
    • Doberman
    • Entelbucher Mountain Dog
    • Flatcoat Retriever
    • French Bulldog
    • German Shorthair
    • Golden Retriever >
      • Golden Retriever Pedigree Charts
    • Irish Water Spaniel >
      • IWS (6 Nov 17)
    • Labrador Retriever
    • Manchester Terrier
    • Mongolian Bankhar >
      • Research Updates
      • Bankhar 1
    • Norwegian Lundehund
    • Plummer Terrier
    • Otterhound
    • Portuguese Water Dog >
      • Portuguese Water Dog (pt 2)
    • Ridgeback
    • Schipperke
    • Standard Poodle >
      • The Problem With Poodles
      • 3poodle pedigree charts
      • 3Poodle Wycliff dogs
      • Poodle Genetics
    • Tibetan Spaniel
    • Tibetan Mastiff
    • West Highland White Terrier
    • Whippet
    • Wirehaired Pointing Griffons
    • UK KC Graphs >
      • UK KC Breed Status
      • UK Groups
      • KC Gundogs
      • KC Hounds
      • KC Terriers >
        • Terriers (select breeds)
      • KC Pastoral
      • KC Toys
      • KC Working
      • KC Utility
      • Australian KC
    • Breed outcrossing programs
  • Resources
    • Genetics Databases
    • Stud Books >
      • American Kennel Club stud books
      • Field Dog stud books
      • The Kennel Club (UK)
    • Learn
    • Videos about dog genetics
    • The Amazing Things Dogs Do! (videos) >
      • Livestock Management
      • Livestock guarding
      • Transportation, exploration, racing
      • Conservation & wildlife management
      • Detection Dogs
      • Medicine & Research
      • Entertainment
      • AKC/CHF Podcasts
    • Read & Watch
    • Bookshelf

The behavior of hot and cold puppies

11/5/2022

 
Carol Beuchat PhD
Back in the good old days, before the development of fancy scientific gadgets that can measure anything to the fifth decimal place, scientists did a lot of good science using very simple, familiar tools. A clever scientist could design experiments that provided information about the neurological basis of body temperature control in animals with nothing more than a heat lamp like the ones you use in your whelping box, and a fan and some water. Ingenuity, creativity, and keen powers of observation were traits of the most successful scientists, much as they still are today in a very different, highly technological context. ​I love reading these old studies. People these days tend to dismiss them because they seem primitive and simple-minded, but a lot of things we know now and take for granted were based on simple experiments cleverly done decades ago.

A good example of one of these was a study by Welker (1959) to learn how puppies are able to stay in contact with their most important heat sources, their mother and their littermates.

The reason you keep a heat source in your whelping box is because newborn puppies are altricial (not fully developed) at birth. Eyes and ears are closed, and they have only two useful senses (temperature and touch). A physiologist would describe a puppy as a "very simple system".
 One consequence of their relative prematurity at birth is that they are unable to generate metabolic heat to control their own body temperature. Although this ability develops over the first few weeks, at birth the puppy behaves pretty much like a water balloon, heating at about the same rate under a heat lamp, and cooling at about the same rate in the cold. Puppies are about 80% water, so the similarity is not surprising.
Physiological systems are very temperature sensitive. Heart rate, respiration, muscle contraction, vision, digestion, nerve impulse transmission, and many other things are affected by temperature; heat makes physiological processes faster, and cold slows them down. You know from personal experience that if you leave your hand in an ice bucket for a few minutes, then try to pick up a penny from the table, not only can you not pick it up, you can't even feel it. Everything is too cold to function.
A cold puppy has the same problems. Respiration is slowed, heart rate and contractility (strength of contraction) declines, digestion stops, and sensory systems don't respond to stimuli. If that wasn't enough, cold muscles mean that the pup can't even move out of the situation to a warmer spot.
Picture
​The other thing the puppy can't do is nurse, which requires integrated activation of both sensory systems (for nipple detection), and the muscles to keep the pup in position and provide the mouth movements necessary to express milk. A very cold puppy in this situation is doomed unless it manages to get to a heat source
At the other extreme is heat, which physiological systems don't cope with very well. Puppies can suffer heat stroke just like an adult dog, causing tissue damage and ultimately shut-down of the physiological systems necessary for life. An adult dog has some ability to prevent heat stroke by increasing heat loss and limiting gain (e.g, panting). But the newborn puppy enters the world with few response options beyond trying to crawl to a suitable place. ​
Of course, the best way of prevent being exposed to a dangerously high or low temperature is for the puppy to avoid it in the first place. We would expect puppies to be pretty good at doing this (after all, the unsuccessful could die), but how?

Back in the 1950s, a scientist named Wexler at the University of Wisconsin did some simple but clever experiments to learn about how newborn puppies can avoid hot and cold to maintain a body temperature suited to their physiology. He used a 250 W infrared heat lamp to produce "warm" and "hot" conditions simply by holding it closer (about 1-2 ft away) or a bit farther away (about 4 ft)  from the puppy. For a cold condition, he moistened the skin of the puppy with water and used a fan to produce evaporative cooling. This was crude; the experimental conditions were subjective and qualitative, and probably not even repeatable. But it was a simple experiment that anyone could do (both then and now) if you had a heat lamp, a glass of water, a fan, and a puppy.

There were several questions Wexler wanted to address.
  • Are puppies able to sense their own body temperate? If they can't do this, they can't take actions to regulate it. This would require the puppy to have a central heat sensor that communicates with the physiological systems involved in a response.
 
  • Can puppies sense hot and cold objects in their environment? A puppy in a place that's too hot needs to know if it's moving towards a place that is coolerr, and likewise for its response to cold.
 
  • Finally all of these must work together to produce the appropriate response by the puppy to get it from a dangerous place to a safe one.
Here's the setup. Wexler worked with 45 mongrel puppies that were 1-3 days old. He had a surface to put one or several puppies on (a table covered with a towel), and the tools for producing hot and cold temperatures (lamp, fan, and water).

The hot and cold conditions were applied until the puppy produced vocal and behavioral responses. The  effects of touching an object just using the fingers applied to various places on the body.

The Puppy Huddle
​
To address the first question about whether puppies can sense their own body temperature, Wexler put a group of 4 puppies on the table. Under cold conditions, the puppies gathered to form a huddle. After some time in the cold, the puppies became agitated and vocalized, but they only moved around within the pile and not away from it. If a heat lamp was directed at the pile of puppies, they became quiet within a few seconds. If the heat lamp was moved closer, vocalizations and movement began again in response to the higher temperature, and the puppies gradually moved apart from the huddle. When the puppies were all separated from each other to avoid the heat from the lamp, the lamp was turned off and activity and vocalizations stopped. The puppies would gradually cool until once again they got cold enough to stimulate  vocalization and movement. This phenomenon worked so well that the puppies could be induced to huddle and disperse over and over, simply by turning the heat lamp on and off. 

This demonstrated two things - first, that the puppies could sense their own body temperature and, second, that they could also moderate their response to body temperature with a skin sensor that could detect both pressure when in contact with littermates, and temperature; that is, whether an object was hot or cold.
​
The Single Puppy

​The behavior of individual puppies in response to heat or cold was even more interesting. 

A lone puppy in the cold sweeps its head from side to side, emitting a cry with each respiration, and occasionally moves forward a short distance. Similar side to side movements of the head occur under hot conditions, again with cries on respiration. While it might take 30 to 60 seconds with the heat lamp turned on to simulate the head movements and vocalizations, both stopped almost immediately when the lamp was turned off. If the lamp was turned on again, the response of the puppy was almost immediate, in contrast to the slower response with the first exposure to heat. This would require thermal sensors on the skin that stimulate a central receptor almost immediately. 
Wexler found that, in the cold, simply touching a puppy could elicit a striking behavior response. A cold puppy would stay more or less in the same place while crying and moving about, but with the touch of two fingers on either side of the tip of the nose, the puppy would move forward quickly, about 3.5 ft in only 15 seconds. This did not happen unless the touch was bilateral, with fingers on the sides of the tip of the nose. Bilateral touching of the sides of the head (instead of the tip of the nose) elicited movement forward, but to a lesser degree. Touching on the neck or body had no effect. If the touch was only on one side (unilateral contact), it caused the puppy to turn in the direction of the stimulus but with no forward progression. 
When a puppy was in hot conditions, it vocalized and swung its head from side to side as it did when cold, but touching the nose with a finger caused the puppy to pull away and turn the head away. With bilateral (2 finger) touch, the puppy might move forward a few inches, but touching other parts of the body produced no response.
Picture
In the trials under hot conditions, the temperature of the fingers mattered. Touching a cool puppy's nose with a warm finger caused the puppy to move forward, while touching both cold and hot puppies with cold fingers stimulated withdrawal and avoidance behavior.

​
This response of newborn puppies to touch was remarkably strong. A puppy could travel a distance of 50 yards in 15 minutes, stimulated simply by a bilateral touch on the nose. And perhaps even more remarkably, the puppies seemed to be as strong after this distance than when they started.

The Integrated Puppy

A newborn puppy might look helpless, with eyes closed and limited ability to move around, but it has remarkable sensory abilities that allow it to maintain some control of its body temperature and avoid extremes that would be dangerous or deadly. We know from simple studies like the one described here that puppies can sense their own body temperature, determine whether it is too high or too low, and take actions to move to a more thermally suitable place. The puppy can sense when it touches something and whether it is hot or cold, and it will move forward in response to a touch on the nose, something that might help it stay with its siblings or mother. 

What might look like random, pointless movements of puppies in your whelping box are actually evidence of the actions a newborn puppy takes to keep its body temperature in a suitable range for growth and physiological functions. Over the next days and weeks, the physiology of the puppy will mature and it will be able to generate and retain enough metabolic heat to maintain a stable body temperature, at which time it is able to become more independent.

​You can learn more about the science of dog breeding in my new online course, "The Science of Canine Husbandry", which is available through the Institute of Canine Biology.

REFERENCES
Welker, WI. 1959. Factors influencing aggregation of neonatal puppies. ​J. Comp. Physiol. Psych., 52(3), 376-380. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0047414

To learn more about the genetics of dogs, check out
ICB's online courses

***************************************

Visit our Facebook Groups

ICB Institute of Canine Biology
...the latest canine news and research

ICB Breeding for the Future
...the science of animal breeding
​

Comments are closed.

    Archives

    January 2030
    February 2025
    January 2025
    December 2024
    November 2024
    October 2024
    July 2024
    June 2024
    May 2024
    March 2024
    February 2024
    July 2023
    April 2023
    November 2022
    July 2022
    May 2022
    April 2022
    March 2022
    February 2022
    November 2021
    October 2021
    December 2020
    January 2020
    August 2019
    July 2019
    June 2019
    May 2019
    April 2019
    March 2019
    February 2019
    January 2019
    December 2018
    November 2018
    September 2018
    August 2018
    July 2018
    June 2018
    May 2018
    October 2017
    August 2017
    May 2017
    April 2017
    March 2017
    February 2017
    January 2017
    December 2016
    November 2016
    September 2016
    August 2016
    July 2016
    June 2016
    April 2016
    March 2016
    February 2016
    January 2016
    December 2015
    November 2015
    October 2015
    September 2015
    August 2015
    July 2015
    June 2015
    May 2015
    April 2015
    March 2015
    January 2015
    December 2014
    November 2014
    October 2014
    September 2014
    August 2014
    July 2014
    June 2014
    May 2014
    February 2014
    December 2013
    October 2013
    September 2013
    July 2013
    March 2013
    July 2012
    April 2012

    Categories

    All
    Behavior
    Border-collie
    Herding

Blog

News


About Us

Contact Us








Copyright © 2012-2017 Institute of Canine Biology
Picture
Picture